Yesterday,
Emily mentioned briefly the science behind seeing, with light entering the eye
and the cones. I didn’t get a chance to
comment, but I had wanted to bring up how some communication scholars are
working with cognitive scientific approaches to perception and visual
engagement. I have a short section in my
thesis that addresses the difference between seeing and perceiving, and thus I’m
going to copy and paste some of the ideas.
Ann Marie Barry proposes a theory of perception that suggests perception
is first an affect and then a mental thought. She remarks that “perception theory” is linked
to “the primacy of emotions in processing all communication, and particularly
targets visual communication as paralleling perceptual process dependent on
primary emotion-based systems of response” (45). Barry challenges the assumption that immediate
visual experience includes any mental conception, and as a Communications
scholar she turns to neurology to illuminate that “sight may indeed begin with
light hitting the retina, [and] vision occurs deep within the brain; . . .
[but] perception, the process by which we derive meaning from what we see, is
an elaborate symphony played first and foremost through the unconscious
emotional system, with neural equipment evolved over millions of years”
(46). Before the conscious recognition
of a visual situation, Barry breaks down the visual experience neurologically:
light/visual information from the environment passes through the cornea, pupil
and lens and hits the receptors in the retina; next, that light/information is
converted into electrical impulses; the optic nerve then communicates these
impulses to the “brain’s visual thalamus and onto the visual cortex where
vision actually occurs” (48). It is in
the thalamus where the electrical impulses divert into two routes: the
thalamo-amygdala pathway and the cortical pathway. The former connects to the amygdale, the
emotional center of the brain; the latter connects to the cortex and begins the
state of awareness. The thalamo-amygdala
route has a shorter distance than the cortical route. Hence, by the time we become aware, our
emotions have already been stimulated by the visual environment, as Barry
notes, “Emotional reactions are therefore faster than conscious ones, and
emotional memory frames all conscious response.
The cortex also sends a second signal to the amygdale, adding conscious
input to emotional reaction and emotional response to thought” (49).
Although
Barry reverts to a cognitive understanding of visual experience, she appears to
express in her perception theory and definition of perception a form of affect
theory. As the emotional center of the
brain gets stimulated from the visual environment, the brain and the body can
act in ways a prior consciousness (Barry 49-55). Barry defines perception as “the process by which we derive meaning through
experience, is a dynamic, interactive system that utilizes built-in genetic
programming to synthesize sensory input, memory, and individual needs. The eyes are only an initial part of the
equation, and can, in fact, be bypassed altogether” (48). In addition to the
eye, the brain is also only one component to perception, and as James J. Gibson
contends, “it is not necessary to assume that anything whatever is transmitted along the optic nerve in the
activity of perception. . . . We can think of vision as a perceptual system,
the brain being simply part of the system” (original emphasis 61). In other
words, perceiving is neither an eye nor a brain-mind centered experience;
rather perceiving is an eye-brain-mind-body affect. Perception is not simply cognitive,
but embodied affects.
Emily
(and AE too) expressed some of these same ideas. In connecting this blog post to my previous, I
find it interesting how Descartes addresses the mind-body problem, in which he
privileges the mind over body, and how I’m addressing the brain, thinking, and
a scientific understanding of experience. Possibly this may be my attraction:
thinking and what happens when we experience something (as well as not
experience something, but simply think).
Barry,
Ann Marie. “Perception Theory.” Handbook
of Visual Communication. Eds. Ken Smith, Sandra Moriarty, Gretchen Barbastsis,
and Keith Kenney. Mahwah: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, Publishers, 2005. 45-62.
Print.
Gibson,
James J. The Ecological Approach to
Visual Perception. 1979. Hillsdale: Lawrence Erlabaum Associates,
Publishers, 1986. Print.